Learning processes : automatic type responses

Training a horse implies the use of positive and negative reinforcements, processes which need to be mastered so that the horse understands what is expected of him, and therefore improves.

2

Technical level :
apprentissage et réponses de type automatique
Table of contents

From « reinforcements » to « automatic » type responses

When a rider is training his horse, he uses a certain number of conditioning situations. For example he teaches the horse to associate striking off to canter with the corresponding aids. When beginning these learning processes the rider should constantly be using « reinfocements ».

The rider can use two types of reinforcement :

  • Either « negative reinforcements », where pressure is applied until the horse performs the desired action ;
  • Or « positive reinforcements », where a reward is given as soon as the horse responds.

At this early stage of the training, if the rider stops using reinforcements (for example the rider stops rewarding the horse) the horse will stop responding. In fact the horse responds in order to obtain a reward, he is said to have a « mental representation of the consequence of the action ». Progressively, during training, as the exercises are repeated the horse becomes mechanized : he will respond to an order through force of habit, without the need for reinforcement on the rider’s part. The horse no longer has a mental representation of the consequences of his action, he no longer acts just to obtain a reward. This form of mechanization  involves a well-known and well documented process in the field of neurosciences : « automatic » type responses, or « habits ». These processes are also involved in many situations, including for us as human beings. For example a pianist forever practising his scales, a sportsman doing the same exercises over and over, or a driver covering the same itinerary every day, all these situations create automatisms. We know that these responses stemming from habit subtend from a specific part of the brain  known as the dorsolateral striatum.

Awareness of these mechanisms in the equine species is important as it enables us to understand why some horses will anticipate more than others the rider’s requests in a dressage test, why they will always turn at the same place, or finish a show jumping course even without the rider. In a series of studies conducted by the Ifce, we sought to determine what factors could influence these processes (Lansade et al., 2017). These studies are based on conditioning protocols, which involve the ideas of « extinction » and « contingency », ideas which are little known, but important to take into account when training horses

EXTINCTION & CONTINGENCY : 2 ideas to be aware of

  • Extinction : when the rider no longer uses any reinforcement whatsoever (e.g he no longer ever gives the horse a reward for a specific action), the horse usually ends up no longer performing the action. The response is said to become extinct.
  • Contingency : this is the likelihood for the horse to obtain a reward for a specific action on his part, compared to receiving the same reward without any action. When rewards are given « free of charge » instead of reserving them for a specific action, the contingency is said to be « degraded ». The consequence is that the horse will be less and less inclined to perform the action. Although this can be used for therapeutical situations in some animals  where we need to do away with unwanted actions. On the other hand, if the trainer has deteriorated the contingency without realising he is doing so, it could lead to problems.

apprentissage et réponses de type automatique
Any training situation with a horse involves conditioning which imply the use of reinforcements. A good knowledge of this process is vital for the horse to understand what is expected of him, and for him to progress © A. Laurioux

Stress and conditioning

apprentissage et réponses de type automatique
Emotive horses are often more apt at developing automatisms. This can be an asset for training, but can also become a factor leading to the development of stereotypies © A. Laurioux.

The fact of learning in stressful conditions, or when the horse develops an emotive side to his character, predisposes him to developing automatic responses. This predisposition is sometimes sought after by riders, as this type of horse becomes mechanized and responds to orders more quickly, without necessarily having to use reinforcements. This could be one of the reasons why top riders appreciate riding emotive horses, even if they are more delicate.


On the negative side, this predisposition could also be linked to the appearance of sterotypical behaviour, which could translate as a higher proportion of emotive horses in this category and could also incite to better catering for the emotive horse’s well-being.

Know more about our authors
  • Translated from french by : Karen DUFFY Translator
  • Léa LANSADE Research engineer in ethology IFCE-INRAE

Bibliography

  • LANSADE L., MARCHAND A., COUTUREAU E. et CALANDREAU L., 2017. Personality and predisposition to form habit behaviours during instrumental conditioning in horses. Plos One, 12(2), 10.1371/journal.pone.0171010.
  • HENRY S. et HAUSBERGER M., 2015. Synthèse sur les influences maternelles de la naissance au sevrage et applications aux conduites d'élevage. 41ème Journée de la Recherche Équine, Institut français du cheval et de l’équitation, pages 93-102.
  • ROCHE H., 2013. Motiver son cheval, clicker traning et récompenses. Editions Belin.
  • SANKEY C., HENRY S., RICHARD-YRIS M.A. et HAUSBERGER M., 2009. Le renforcement comme médiateur de la relation homme/cheval. 35ème Journée de la Recherche Equine, Les Haras Nationaux, pages 89-100.
  • VALENCHON M., LEVY F. et LANSADE L., 2013. Influence du tempérament sur les performances d'apprentissage et de mémoire du cheval : bilan de trois années de travail de thèse. 39ème Journée de la Recherche Equine, Paris, Institut français du cheval et de l’équitation, pages 147-150.
To find this document: www.equipedia.ifce.fr/en
Editing date: 19 05 2024

Was this information sheet useful ?